Boycott the "Greater" Israeli Apartheid Regime!

Boycott the "Greater" Israeli Apartheid Regime!

Învaţă, Cunoaşte-te pe tine însuţi, Schimbă-te... Învaţă de la oameni, Cunoaşte-i, Schimbaţi Împreună Lumea!

Wednesday, 5 January 2011

Radical Psychology - Vol.8, Issue 1


by Virginia Braun and Leonore Tiefer [*]

“[T]oday we have a whole society held in thrall to the drastic plastic of labial rejuvenation” (Elliott, 2008, p. 21)

Following a British Channel 4 documentary on the ‘designer vagina’, screened in 2008, Channel 4’s website for the show ran a poll which asked visitors ‘have you considered having a designer vagina?’ (see http://www.channel4.com/health/microsites/G/g-spot/perfect-vagina/vote.html). Of the 9216 respondents (on 13 Oct, 2008), 43% answered yes, they had considered having such surgery. [1]

While viewers of the site (and thus voters) are likely to have a particular interest in female genital cosmetic surgery (FGCS), the result still indicates an incredibly high proportion of women dislike their genitalia enough to have considered surgery as an option. However, it is not much lower than the proportion of women indicating, in both unscientific and scientific surveys, that they would have cosmetic surgery -- of any sort (e.g., Aitkenhead, 2005; Asthana, 2005; Most women 'want plastic surgery'," 2001; Sarwer et al., 2005). Cosmetic surgery has become a normalised practice within, and beyond, the west (see Blum, 2003; Brooks, 2004; Davis, 2003; Elliott, 2008; Fraser, 2003; Pitts-Taylor, 2007, for various analyses around normalisation), and now constitutes a viable solution to multiple forms of bodily distress for many women, and, increasingly, men.

Cosmetic surgery culture -- which is wrapped up in celebrity culture and fantasies of fame and beauty, as well as consumerism and economic factors (see Blum, 2003; Elliott, 2008) -- invites us into a regime of self-surveillance and technologically-mediated bodily self-improvement; it invites a focus on the minutiae of bodily imperfection. The body has become the starting point for radical self-transformation (Elliott, 2008); if we do not like something, and can afford it, a surgeon can change it (although not always successfully, and often at a [small] risk to life, e.g., Landman, 2004). Cosmetic surgery is material, a carving into flesh; it is also profoundly psychological -- a form of psychotherapy (Fraser, 2003; Gilman, 1998, 1999), aimed at providing an embodied solution to a psychological concern -- the person’s perception of a particular body part, and anxiety and distress that causes. [2] The idea that cosmetic surgery is ‘healthful’ and producing of inner wellbeing is now a common part of discourse around it (Pitts-Taylor, 2007).

Although genital distress is nothing new for women, women’s genitalia were, until recently, largely excluded from the intense self-surveillance and improvement imperatives that cosmetic surgery culture mandates. The ‘designer vagina’ phenomenon has changed that, with a wide range of female genital cosmetic surgery procedures targeting every part of women’s genital anatomy. These procedures aim to alter aesthetics (and/or function). They are performed in the absence of clear pathology, and without an evidence base (Cartwright and Cardozo, 2008; The American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists, 2007; Tracy, 2007), and carry “real risks of potential complications” (The Royal Australian and New Zealand College of Obstetricians and Gynaecologists, 2008). The most common procedures appear to be labiaplasty (reduction of labia minora), and vaginal ‘tightening’. Although still not vastly popular, limited statistics indicate these surgeries are increasing rapidly (Liao and Creighton, 2007, http://www.plasticsurgery.org/).

There is scant reliable information about these procedures. Some clinical case reports exist (e.g., Alter, 1998; Choi and Kim, 2000; Di Saia, 2008; Fliegner, 1997; Giraldo, González, and de Haro, 2004; Hodgkinson and Hait, 1984; Maas and Hage, 2000; Munhoz et al., 2006), mainly focused on technique, and some authors report more than a few cases (de Alencar Felicio, 2007; Pardo, Solà, Ricci, and Guilloff, 2006; Pardo, Solà, Ricci, Guilloff, and Freundlich, 2006; Rouzier, Louis-Sylvestre, Paniel, and Haddad, 2000), but there is not much beyond that. Some discussion about ethics is starting to appear, in limited form (e.g., Cartwright and Cardozo, 2008; Cosmetic Labiaplasty: The Great Ethical Debate," 2007; Goodman et al., 2007). [3] By far the most ‘extensive’ literature on the topic is critical (feminist) commentary and research (e.g., Adams, 1997; Braun, 2005, 2006, 2009a, 2009b; Davis, 2002; Green, 2005; Liao and Creighton, 2007; McNamara, 2006; Sullivan, 2007; Tiefer, 2008a).

In this paper, we critically examine FGCS in the west, locating the practice and process within broader structural, political, sociocultural and economic factors and practices. We argue that the practice and discourse of female genital cosmetic surgery works to promote one genital aesthetic as ‘right’, and simultaneously pathologise genital diversity in women. Rather than only discussing this surgery theoretically and academically, as previous critiques have done, we identify a range of factors and interventions to disrupt this trend and the concurrent pathologising of the genitalia. The broad framework we draw on is from critical health psychology (e.g., Murray, 2004; Stainton Rogers, 1996). Rather than the individualistic focus of mainstream health psychology (Campbell, 2004), critical health psychology instead adopts a more constructionist approach (Stainton Rogers, 1996), and theorises health and illness as intrinsically socially and politically located (Stam, 2004). This suggests an analysis of the role of economic, political and social factors, both locally and globally, is crucially important in understanding, theorising and analysing health (Murray and Campbell, 2003); it also locates a more diverse array of targets beyond the individual in interventions for change.


Bodies of Difference: Pathology and Perfection

To note that (western) women often have problematic relationships with their bodies is to state the obvious. A significant majority of women see their bodies as inadequate in some way (e.g., Harris and Carr, 2001), failing in the quest for feminine bodily perfection, or even ‘adequacy’. The cultural idealisation of a certain (typically unrealistic) bodily form is a key factor in making female embodiment problematic for many women. But within the range of diversity of female bodies, certain have been further marked as pathologically different -- for instance fat, non-white or ‘inappropriately’ sexual bodies (Gilman, 1985; Terry, 1990) -- and thus as inherently suspect, irredeemable in that particular embodiment.

Within consumer culture, women’s bodies occupy a location as commodity (Haiken, 1997; Negrin, 2002), but they are not singular commodity units; women’s bodies have long been “dissected into physical parts” (Duke and Kreshel, 1998, p. 49), and diversity and pathology extend to particular body parts as well as to whole bodies. The pathologisation of certain (non-white) facial features -- and surgical ‘correction’ thereof -- has a long but also changing and variable history (e.g., Gilman, 1999; Kaw, 1998; Pitts-Taylor, 2007; Preminger, 2001). A relatively recent example of this shift from bodily variation to ‘pathological’ difference can be illustrated with reference to breasts. In the 1950s, the small breast -- which at certain times in western history has been the most aesthetically desired -- became a ‘deformity’; women suffered the newly created medical condition of ‘hypomastia’ (Hausmann, 1992, cited in Adams, 1997), which persists to this day -- the solution: augmentation mammaplasty, the currently most popular form of cosmetic surgery (now typically for ‘inadequate’ rather than ‘absent’ breasts).

This same shift to pathologising diversity is now occurring for women’s genitalia. Here, we can identify a similar pathologisation through language, with the appearance of ‘hypertropic’ labia minora. Women’s genitals show broad diversity in all aspects (Lloyd, Crouch, Minto, Liao, and Creighton, 2005), and visible labia minora are statistically ‘normal’ and probably more common than invisible ones (Corinna, n.d.). Yet labia minora are identified as hypertrophic if they ‘protrude’ beyond the labia majora to a certain degree. How much is a matter of much debate, with definitions touted -- with no apparent evidence base --ranging from 2cm to 5cm (see de Alencar Felicio, 2007; Goodman et al., 2007; Maas and Hage, 2000; Pardo et al., 2006; Rouzier et al., 2000).

A description of ‘protrusion’ invokes abnormality, but the label/diagnosis of ‘hypertrophy’ locates certain genital appearance firmly within the realm of the medical and the pathological. The language is evident in medical discourse, but is also being used by women (and men) to identify particular morphologies: for instance, various girls on a recent online discussion board identified that “I have hypertrophic labia minora” (See http://www.steadyhealth.com/Is_hypertrophy_of_labia_minora_normal_thing__t53844.html).

In taking up such language, a certain (typically normal) bodily variation is medicalised and pathology ensured. And with the willing uptake of this condition, a surgical solution is implicitly rationalised and justified, over other possible ‘solutions’ -- because this becomes a real and authentic (material) problem, it needs to be resolved.

A pathological interpretation of women’s genitalia is, however, hardly new, and a continuation of a long history of western pathologising of women’s reproductive (and sexual) bodies more generally (e.g., see Ussher, 2006). Over the last few centuries, the genitalia of women deemed inherently suspect either by race (e.g., Black women) or by sexuality (e.g., lesbian women; prostitutes) have been subject to scientific scrutiny. From the 19th and 20th Century European public display of the genitalia of the Southern African Khoikhoi woman, Sartje Baartman, with her elongated labia (the so called ‘hottentot apron’), to the 19th and 20th Century measurements of the vulvas of prostitutes and lesbian women (Gilman, 1985; Somerville, 1997; Terry, 1990, 1995) to determine pathological difference, inappropriate feminine embodiment and (hyper) sexuality were read from genital ‘excess’. In this new ‘scientific’ diagnosis, it is still an excess of tissue that results in ‘hypertrophic’ labia minora -- the ‘too fleshy’ vulva (Davis, 2002) remains problematic. This troublesome real-life fleshiness fits with theorising of the (female sexual) body as abject (Kristeva, 1982), a deeply problematic, fluid, messy, seeping body that furnishes no clear boundary for the self, rendering subjectivity problematic. While the abject has often focused on fluidity -- Grosz (1994) argued, for instance, that “women’s corporeality is inscribed as a mode of seepage” (p. 203) --a similar analysis can apply here, where the offending article is tissue not ‘neatly’ contained with a seamless corporeal boundary. In Covino’s (2004) analysis of abjection and cosmetic surgery, cosmetic surgery is a practice that ‘amends’ the abject body; in FGCS, the ‘messy’ labial tissue is replaced with the ‘clean slit’ (Davis, 2002, p. 12).

Labia reduction appears to return the vulva to a pre-pubescent state (Fitzpatrick, 2007; Liao and Creighton, 2007; Manderson, 2004); arguably, to a pre-sexual body. This might appear an odd claim, as FGCS is often directly marketed around the improvement to one’s sexual life that will ensue (Braun, 2005). But perhaps here we are seeing a process not too dissimilar to the measurements of the labia of lesbian women, or even the western 19th and 20th Century clitoridectomies on young girls (Green, 2005; Rodriguez, 2008; Sheehan, 1997). Perhaps this, too, is about the policing or production of appropriate embodied (hetero)sexuality for women, but now appropriate sexuality is understood by women as accessed through particular genital morphology. Although Manderson (2004) suggests that ‘messy’ labia minora are associated with ‘promiscuous’ women, and these are stigmatised (see also Bramwell, Morland, and Garden, 2007), we wonder if something quite different is happening. It appears that ‘long’ labia (and indeed a ‘loose’ vagina) signal, to some women, a body ‘unfit’ for -- undeserving of -- sexual activity, and, even, of questionable womanhood (see Braun and Wilkinson, 2005, for discussion of links between genitals and gendered identity). In a context in which there is a “obsession with sexual gratification” (Hart and Wellings, 2002, p. 899) and sexual pleasure is framed almost as an individual’s right (see Braun, 2005, for more on this), this is a tenuous position to occupy, rendering surgery a legitimate avenue down which seek what is both entitlement and obligation (Hawkes, 1996) -- a sexual body.

While the too-present vulva -- hypertrophic labia -- and the absent breast discussed earlier -- hypomastia -- might appear different, a very similar logic is evident in the construction of both ‘deformities.’ Based on the premise of gendered dimorphism, each identifies as pathological a body that blurs the boundaries between distinctly ‘female’ or distinctly ‘male’: hypomastia renders the chest potentially ‘male’; ‘too long’ labia render the vulva open to a male reading (see also McNamara, 2006):

“My inner labial lips were long, messy and flappy and, worst of all, they hung down outside the outer lips. It looked like I had a small penis dangling down and I couldn’t stand to see myself naked” [Company Magazine, UK, 2003]

“I was so worried I thought I might not actually be fully female at one point, like the labia was actually meant to develop into balls but didn’t quite get there” [‘Flappyflaps, posted 03/09/2008 on http://answers.polldaddy.com/viewPoll.aspx?view=resultsandid=848800]

Similarly, ‘excess’ labia majora tissue is also viewed through such a lens:

“The outer layer can become almost scrotal, very wrinkly and lax,’ Dr. Romanzi said.” (Singer, 2008)

These accounts are not so far removed from Havelock Ellis’ identification of the labia majora of a lesbian woman as resembling “fleshy sacs” (Ellis, 1915, cited in Somerville, 1997, p. 42). Somerville notes that “the ‘fleshy sacs’ of this woman … invoked the anatomy of a phantom male body inhabiting the lesbian’s anatomical features” (1997, p. 42). In interpreting certain female genital morphology as ‘masculine’, these accounts reveal the societal importance of visible gender dimorphism. This is equally evident in the past -- and even current -- surgical alterations of the genitals of intersex infants. Ann Fausto-Sterling (2000), for instance, highlights the ‘unacceptable’ grey area between the acceptable size (<1cm) of a clitoris and the acceptable size (>2.5cm) of a penis in a newborn. The logic of dimorphism is evident also if you consider FGCS in comparison to male genital cosmetic surgery. In the former, reduction of tissue and ‘size’ is paramount; in the latter, the production of a larger penis is key (Cochrane, 2007).

These accounts also, importantly, reveal the limits of knowledge of women’s genital diversity, and of imagination around what women’s genital diversity can (or should) be. In our sociocultural imaginary, it seems that female genitalia occupy a morphology far more limited that their physiological reality (as demonstrated by Blank, 1993; Lloyd et al., 2005). And this discrepancy, between material flesh and imaginary morphology, can produce considerable psychological distress for some women (e.g., see numerous entries on http://answers.polldaddy.com/viewPoll.aspx?view=resultsandid=848800). The website Scarleteen reports ‘what’s wrong with my labia?’ has become one of the most asked questions in recent years, and that they’ve “been seeing more and more -- often unfounded -- worries about labia as the years have gone by” (Corinna, n.d., para 1). A key point to make is that how we ‘come to know’, ‘see’ and indeed read body parts as flesh, and beyond that, how these are personally and socially meaningful, depends on culture, on the discursive contexts which render visible and invisible various different objects and meanings (Pitts-Taylor, 2007).

Given the normalisation of cosmetic surgery, one concern we have is that for women who already have significant genital concerns, surgery will come to occupy a position in which it is seen as the only solution to the distress. Any other potential approach to treat this psychologically (let alone socio-politically) is taken out of the equation. What this does is not only undermine other non-surgical ‘treatments’ and promote FGCS -- a general feature of medicalisation processes (Doyle, 2007). It also naturalises, normalises and individualises the experience of genital distress for women, locating the problems she perceives as an almost inevitable response to unappealing physiology. What is unappealing becomes almost as given (e.g., who could possibly like ‘long’ labia minora?) and her response is located legitimately within herself/psychology because of this. Any social influence on her aesthetics and desires disappear. Aesthetics become a matter of (almost) the obvious, rather than the socially constructed. With the numerous before-and-after photos on surgeon websites or occasional clinical case reports of labiaplasty, for instance, we are expected to naturally agree that the ‘after’ shot is an improvement on the ‘before’. This undermines the possibility of ‘undoing’ such aesthetics or perceptions through other levels of interventions, be they psychologically or socially oriented.

A second important concern is that with the (re)pathologisation of genital diversity for women, through the promotion of certain ‘desirable’ vulval and vaginal states, any partially formed or vague anxieties that women might already have could be enhanced, and develop into more significant ‘distress’ about their vulva. Another possibility is that women who had no concern about their vulval appearance might suddenly pay attention to, and dislike, their vulvas if they differ from the ideal “clean slit” (Davis, 2002, p. 12) of FGCS discourse and imagery. It could be that “a brand-new worry is being created” (Davis, 2002, p. 8), and that things like advertising and media coverage produce a demand for FGCS (Liao and Creighton, 2007). This is in line with Elliott’s (2008) argument that “cosmetic surgery culture promotes the very anxieties it seeks to quell” (p. 90). The consumer system relies, of course, on a perpetual cycle involving the (personal) identification of deficiencies, and the consumption of offered up (temporary) solutions (Bordo, 1997).

But FGCS did not emerge in isolation, and likewise women’s (potentially increasing) genital concerns have not appeared in isolation. In line with a critical health psychology approach, which moves beyond the individual to consider contextual influences on women’s perceptions and desires around their sexual and reproductive bodies (e.g., see Bordo, 1993; Martin, 1987; Ussher, 2006), numerous (gendered) contexts need to be considered if we are fully to understand this practice, and even the desire for it in the first place. We now briefly discuss various continuities between these concerns and FGCS practices, from a range of different perspectives, before considering interventions and activism to disrupt the pathologisation of female genital diversity.


Continuities for Genital Distress and Female Genital Cosmetic Surgery

Women’s perceptions and experiences of their genitalia are far from straightforward, positive, or even neutral (e.g., see Berman, Berman, Miles, Pollets, and Powell, 2003; Bramwell and Morland, 2008; Braun and Wilkinson, 2003, 2005; Ellis, 2006; Ensler, 1998; Green, 2005; Morrison, Bearden, Ellis, and Harriman, 2005; Reinholtz and Muehlenhard, 1995). Numerous concerns, often related to appearance, are expressed by women, and “many women nurture fearful fantasies about the abnormality of their genitals” (Laws, 1987, p. 9). This reflects the wider sociocultural context (Braun and Wilkinson, 2001), in which “pudendal disgust is a social reality” (Tiefer, 2008a, p. 475). A diverse range of negative sociocultural genital meanings persist in women’s everyday contexts -- for instance in slang, in which visible labia minora are pathologised through crude slang such as “beef curtains” and “fishy fanny flaps” (Braun and Kitzinger, 2001). In such contexts, it is not surprising that women experience genital ‘concern’.

More specific, and newer, shifts in cultural norms also help make sense of the ‘rise’ of the designer vagina. Two central factors in western cultures relate to pubic hair removal and pornography -- evident in this woman’s comment related to an online labiaplasty poll: “I blame porn for this. And Brazilian waxes. Nobody was comparing lips when they were nicely hidden in the bush” (ClatieK, posted 30 October 2008, http://www.thefrisky.com/post/246-poll-would-you-ever-snip-down-below/). The emphasis on removal of most or all pubic hair for women has become widespread in the last 5-10 years (e.g., Fitzpatrick, 2007; Peixoto Labre, 2002; Tilley, 2007), to the point it has been claimed as “routine” (Trager, 2006, p. 117) and “entirely mainstream” (Cochrane, 2007, p. 30) in adults and adolescents -- so much so that some women are reportedly having ‘permanent Brazilians’ (McLean, 2007). The removal of most or all pubic hair makes the vulva more visible (Green, 2005), and more an object of attention (and also more prepubescent, Peixoto Labre, 2002). It is indicative of a shift from the vulva being a part of women’s ‘natural’ body where modification was not mandated, to being a part almost inherently inadequate without at least some minor modificatory (e.g., depilatory) practice. FGCS is part of this shift in status. Cochrane (2007) notes a link between this trend for pubic hair removal, and “the rise and rise of vaginal cosmetic surgery” (p. 30); and it is frequently noted in women’s narratives of FGCS presented in the media:

When it became all the rage to get heavy bikini waxes and have almost no pubic hair, my prominent labia really started to bother me (Marie Claire, UK, 2000)

This hairless norm has also been linked to pornography (Peixoto Labre, 2002), as has FGCS, and especially to magazines such as Playboy: “by 2001, pubic hair seems to almost have disappeared from the pages of [Playboy]” (Peixoto Labre, 2002, p. 120). In recent years, ‘mainstream’ pornography has come to occupy a more normalised position within western heterosexual relationships (Häggström-Nordin, Sandberg, Hanson, and Tydén, 2006), so that heterosexual women may well be exposed to a range of (depilated) supposedly particularly desirable vulvas, to which they can compare their own. In media accounts, women locate these vulvas as desirable:

I really wanted my genitals to have the tight, tucked-in look that I’d seen in men’s magazines (Cosmopolitan, Aus/NZ, 2001)

Numerous sources indicate women or take porn images to surgeons to demonstrate the vulva they want (Green, 2005; Liao and Creighton, 2007):

Women sometimes come armed, says [surgeon] Martin Rees, with a porn magazine, a variant on taking a picture of some celeb to the hairdressers” (Metro, NZ, 2005)

[Lisa’s] deciding exactly what looks she’s going to plump for. But she’s not looking at beautiful bodies and faces in a fashion magazine; she’s looking at full-frontal shots of vaginas in Playboy. (Shine, AUS/NZ, 2001)

Broader contemporary cultural influences related to women’s genital perceptions, desires and practices include marketing by surgeons and medicalisation. Medicalisation is a process whereby ‘problems’ -- which might already ‘exist’ or be newly created -- are located within the biological, and the influence of the social or psychological are ignored or downplayed. Solutions to these problems similarly prioritise the biomedical at the expense of the psychological or social (Conrad, 2007). In the final section of the paper, where we discuss action and activism against FGCS, we situate our analysis within broader processes like medicalisation and disease-mongering, as well as advertising and media representation.


Action and Activism against Female Genital Cosmetic Surgery: A New View Perspective

FGCS is clearly a problematic group of practices, and the practice and discourse around them (re)pathologises women’s genital diversity, potentially creating a self-referential cycle in which surgery becomes more and more popular and diverse genital morphology less and less acceptable. While academic critique of this nature is vital, it is simply not enough; public and political level action is needed to intervene to challenge this process. One area of female genital cutting where this has occurred is in relation to practices commonly referred to female genital mutilation (‘FGM’) (which tend to be distinguished from (western) FGCS by a rhetoric of choice, Braun, 2009a). [4] In contrast, challenging the practice and growth of FGCS is a new area for feminist activism. Before discussing activism against FGCS, we briefly survey some activism against ‘FGM’, as well as activism aimed at intervening in other practices which promote one bodily singular ideal or norm and problematise embodied diversity and difference.

Various feminists from around the globe have campaigned against ‘FGM’ on every continent and in every medium and venue for many years. As a consequence, many countries have banned it (see Rahman and Toubia, 2000). A widespread international coalition of activists as well as institutional agents such as the UN, WHO, and the European Parliament work to bring the issues before the public in a variety of forms (see http://www.un.org/News/Press/docs/2008/dsgsm377.doc.htm). The goal of anti-‘FGM’ activism is to reduce the practices both through legal prohibitions and via raising the level of public disapproval (e.g., Olembo, 2007). The key discursive moves in the global anti-‘FGM’ campaign were, first, to include FGM within the rubric of “violence against women” and, second, to reframe violence against women as a human rights issue , as one of a number of ‘harmful cultural practices’ (Jeffreys, 2005). These shifts, which occurred over a period of years towards the end of the twentieth century, expanded the discussion of FGM beyond the frame of “gender and health,” allowing large and well-funded human rights organizations such as Amnesty International to become involved (e.g., http://www.amnesty.org/en/library/asset/ACT77/002/2000/en/dom-ACT770022000en.pdf). [5]

Another area of much more limited activism in the west, but one which has resulted in some success, is around the medical/surgical practices for intersex infants. Arguing for diversity and against pseudobiological binaries, feminists have challenged the involvement of genital surgery in the management of intersex individuals since the 1970s (Kessler and McKenna, 1978). Intersex individuals themselves began to advocate in an organised fashion against genital surgeries in the US in 1993, with the founding of The Intersex Society of North America (ISNA) (http://isna.org; see also Hegarty and Chase, 2000). Partly as a result of activists' research, education, and advocacy, a new standard of care was published in the medical literature in 2006 representing negotiations among many stakeholders in this debate (Lee et al., 2006). [6] However, medical authority that stresses the reality of a two sex model with surgical "correction" for "deviations" is still dominant in this area (Karkazis, 2008).

Another related area of feminist activism can be seen around what we might call western ‘body politics.’ For example, there have been numerous challenges to the narrow range of media portrayals of women’s bodies in advertising and other media (e.g., Bordo, 1993; Gill, 2008; Kilbourne, 1999). Many public health campaigners have raised awareness of commercialized “beauty practices” as a source of danger and oppression (e.g., http://phsj.org/?page_id=10), and this has also been the focus of feminist academic/activist critique (Jeffreys, 2005). Finally, although space precludes a detailed focus, there has been a long feminist history of activism related to women’s health issues, including extensive activism around breast implant surgery (for a timeline and links, see http://www.commandtrust.com/implants.html).

Although critical feminist commentary about FGCS has appeared in scholarly, print media and online articles, as well as in more informal online blogs and commentaries (e.g., Anna, 2007; Corinna, n.d.), there are as yet only a few indications of feminist activism. This absence of activism perhaps reflects the dominance of choice rhetoric related to western bodily practices (see Braun, 2009a), and possibly a reduction in activism more generally. Thus far activism seems to be against the invisibility of women’s genital diversity, rather than against surgical interventions, per se. For example, the ‘Everyday Bodies Project,’ started in 2004, posts self-photographed images of women’s genitals to a signed-up online community, to combat ignorance and concerns about ‘abnormality,’ and to challenge the typically uniform (edited) genital imagery most available to women (Parker, 2007). Similarly, the author of the pro-vulva website Vagina Vérité is planning to produce a book of vulval images to “make it clear that diversity is the norm” (http://www.vaginaverite.com/book.html). And sex educators who focus their attention on adolescents have been especially vocal about the need for positive attitudes towards their genitalia, especially as teens can be influenced by TV surgery “makeover” shows and pro-surgery articles and imagery in magazines (Corinna, n.d.; Redd, 2008), and increasingly express concerns about vulval normality, as discussed above.

Our work has focused on FGCS as part of a wider critique of the medicalisation of women’s bodies and sexualities and the creation of new homogeneities and new norms for sexual function and experience. It is situated within The New View Campaign (see http://www.newviewcampaign.org/), convened in 1999 by a group of feminist social scientists, academics and health care providers in reaction to the escalated medicalisation of women’s sexual ‘problems’ that followed the blockbuster success of Viagra (Tiefer, 2008b). Most of The New View’s work to date has focused on analyzing and publicizing the harms to women, and to the understanding of sexuality, from the new sexuality medications and medical framings of sexual life (see http://newviewcampaign.org/). The Campaign based our analysis of harm on similar feminist critiques arising from scandals about sex hormones (HRT, National Women’s Health Network, 2002 ), dangerous contraceptives (Dalkon shield, Hicks, 1994 ), and birthing practices (unnecessary hysterectomies, Morgen, 2002 ).

Ten years on, The New View Campaign’s perspective on the medicalisation of sexuality has expanded greatly. In our discussions of “the hunt for the pink Viagra,” for example, instead of focusing attention solely on urologists and other physicians as usurpers of sex research and women’s sexual health care, we now see them as overt or disguised agents who promote the profit-seeking agenda of a global pharmaceutical industry. We now understand that the influence of the pharmaceutical industry grows because it benefits from favourable governmental and professional organization regulations regarding advertising, education and drug approval (Tiefer, 2008b). Over these 10 years, our understanding of medicalisation and sexuopharmaceuticalisation has moved beyond simply targeting doctors and drug company representatives to include wider networks of influence including political, media, corporate, educational, and professional interests. Doctors and drug companies alone would be unable to accomplish hegemony over research and professional education practices without the active collusion of these wider networks (Tiefer, 2008b).

A new social movement has grown to challenge the public health impact of various corporate practices (Freudenberg, 2005). Academics and non-profit reform groups have revealed how, in their pursuit of profits, corporations use advertising, public relations, and lobbying to defeat or water down progressive regulations that would protect the health of the public. For example, instead of banning cigarette ads entirely, laws only ban them in newspapers, some magazines and television, while permitting ads on billboards and other magazines. In addition to academic and theoretical critique, activism for change has involved these ‘reformers’ challenging the unhealthy policies and practices of corporations by using the very same tactics: media advertising, public relations and lobbying to influence regulation, legislation, and research.

The concept of disease-mongering [7] emerged as an important tool for analyzing how the sexual medicine industry (which is larger than just the pharmaceutical industry) was manipulating the media and public opinion to create markets for a medicalised view of sexual problems (Moynihan and Cassels, 2005). The New View Campaign has examined how the ‘research’ and ‘educational’ activities of the pharmaceutical industry have themselves become disease-mongering tactics used to market drug products (cf Payer, 1992). This involves practices such as: taking a normal function and implying that there is something wrong with it, and it should be treated; assuming suffering that isn’t necessarily there; defining as large a proportion of the population as possible as suffering from the ‘disease’; using an end point in clinical research that advantages the pharmaceutical company rather than being important to the public; promoting technology as risk-free magic (Tiefer, 2006). Most of these are easily applicable to FGCS.

The New View Campaign’s activism over these years to challenge Big Pharma’s disease-mongering tactics has consisted of many journalism interviews, public lectures, debates at medical congresses, public testimony at government hearings, academic publications, self-published brochures and teaching manuals, a website and listserv, letters to government agencies and professional continuing education courses (Tiefer, 2008b). This multi-pronged approach has been a key strength of the Campaign, which has been successful, for instance in helping defeat the first sex drug for women that came to the US Food and Drug Administration in 2004.

The Campaign’s interest in FGCS came about because of a sudden escalation in promotion of this new group of genital surgery procedures through internet videos and television “makeover” shows. The largest impetus seemed to come from one particular Los Angeles gynaecologist who has developed a franchise-like arrangement for training doctors in trademarked procedures using patented equipment and licensed media ad campaigns. The combination of medicalisation for a new ‘condition‘ (distress over clitoral, labial and vaginal appearance), and the flagrant use of business practices which cross the boundaries of ethical advertising practice (see The American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists, 2006), provoked a strong reaction from the New View group. We came to believe that, despite claims that they are about empowering women and improving women’s sexual pleasure (see Braun, 2005), these surgeries were being recommended out of a misogynist disregard for women’s genital diversity and a willingness to exploit women’s lack of knowledge and confidence about their genitals. This called for action.

A small New York-based feminist group studied the journalism about FGCS, the websites of the FGCS providers, the complex and extensive feminist literature on cosmetic surgery, writings about ‘FGM’, and the New View pro-diversity and anti-medicalisation philosophy, and developed a campaign to challenge FGCS.
The goals of the campaign are:

To create public concern about the unchecked expansion of the FGCS industry and its lack of scientific research support;

To pressure professional OB-GYN and plastic surgeons’ associations to collect data on these procedures, and to censure or sanction FGCS surgeons who offer services without publishing research outcomes;

To expand the idea of ’informed consent‘ for FGCS to include genital education about anatomical diversity through showing independent illustrative and scientific materials;

To shed light on the growth of a new set of medical business practices that uses franchise models, public relations, multiple advertising avenues, and all the bells and whistles of contemporary marketing to medicalise everyday bodies, lives, and functioning.

To achieve these goals, we designed an activist campaign consisting of:

Development of a website with resources for press and public (http://www.newviewcampaign.org/fgcs.asp);

Letters to medical and governmental agencies and organizations calling for increased consumer protection, and increased professional standards;

A 2-hour sidewalk rally in front of a NYC FGCS surgeon’s office (including guerrilla theater);

Distribution of a press release announcing the sidewalk rally;

Collection of a list of individual and organizational endorsers of the rally;

Sharing of resources with feminist university organizations to educate their members about the issues;

Collaboration with documentarians interested in FGCS.

The public rally was attended by about 30 feminists and photos can be seen on http://www.newviewcampaign.org/fgcs.asp. Major press coverage included an excellent article in the largest American newsweekly, Time magazine (Fitzpatrick, 2008). The resources will remain online indefinitely. In upcoming conference presentations we plan to use this activist event as a model for feminists planning public actions to challenge the continuing pathologisation of women’s bodies and sexualities.


Conclusion: The ‘Personal’ Remains Political

With this paper we have aimed to present both academic critique of the emergence of FGCS along with activism to challenge the practice. The case of FGCS demonstrates, once again, the feminist claim that ‘the personal is political,’ and that this concept is still as relevant as when it was first stated. We have outlined cultural factors which make it entirely understandable that women both experience anxiety and distress about their genitalia, and seek cosmetic surgery to change them. Women’s personal distress and desires around genitalia need, however, to be located within a wider socio-political context, which includes active practices of medicalisation as well as pathologisation of genital diversity through the promotion of one ideal vulval form. As a final point, it is important to realise that it is not just women who are influenced by culture, economics and practices such as medicalisation. Health and other related professionals are cultural members as well as professionals; their practices can reflect, and reinforce, negative cultural assumptions around women’s genitals, sometimes in quite subtle ways (e.g., Kapsalis, 1997; Lomax and Casey, 1998; Pliskin, 1995). For this reason, comprehensive critical education around genital diversity and normality remains essential.

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------
References:


Abusharaf, R. M. (2001). Virtuous cuts: Female genital circumcision in an African ontology. Differences, 12(1), 112-140.
Adams, A. (1997). Molding women's bodies: The surgeon as sculptor. In D. S. Wilson and C. M. Laennec (Eds.), Bodily discursions: Gender, representations, technologies (pp. 59-80). New York: State University of New York Press.
Aitkenhead, D. (2005). Most British women now expect to have cosmetic surgery in their lifetime. How did the ultimate feminist taboo become just another lifestyle choice? The Guardian, (14 September). Retrieved from http://www.guardian.co.uk/gender/story/0,11812,1569428,00.html
Allotey, P., Manderson, L., and Grover, S. (2001). The politics of female genital surgery in displaced communities. Critical Public Health, 11(3), 189-201.
Alter, G. J. (1998). A new technique for aesthetic labia minora reduction. Annals of Plastic Surgery, 40(3), 287-290.
Angelmar, R., Angelmar, S., and Kane, L. (2007). Building strong condition brands. Journal of Medical Marketing, 7(4), 341-351.
Anna. (2007). Pimp my vadge: The pornographer's opinion. Jezebel.com Retrieved 11 November, 2008, from http://jezebel.com/gossip/clinical-trials/pimp-my-vadge-the-pornographers-opinion-267049.php
Asthana, A. (2005). Half of British women would consider plastic surgery. The Observer. Retrieved from http://www.guardian.co.uk/uk/2005/sep/25/health.gender
Berman, L., Berman, J., Miles, M., Pollets, D., and Powell, J. A. (2003). Genital self-image as a component of sexual health: Relationship between genital self-image, female sexual function and quality of life measures. Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 29(s), 11-21.
Blank, J. (1993). Femalia. San Francisco: Down There Press.
Blum, V. L. (2003). Flesh wounds: The culture of cosmetic surgery. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Bordo, S. (1993). Unbearable weight: Feminism, western culture, and the body. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Bordo, S. (1997). Twilight Zones: The hidden life of cultural images from Plato to O.J. Berkeley, CA: University of California Press.
Bramwell, R., and Morland, C. (2008). Genital appearance satisfaction in women: the development of a questionnaire and exploration of correlates. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, First article, 1-13.
Bramwell, R., Morland, C., and Garden, A. S. (2007). Expectations and experience of labial reduction: A qualitative study. BJOG: An International Journal of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 114(12), 1493-1499.
Braun, V. (2005). In search of (better) female sexual pleasure: Female genital 'cosmetic' surgery. Sexualities, 8(4), 407-424.
Braun, V. (2006). Making sense of female genital alteration practices. Making the Connection, 4(3), online: http://www.siecus.org/inter/connection/conn0060.html
Braun, V. (2009a). “The women are doing it for themselves”: The rhetoric of choice and agency around female genital ‘cosmetic surgery'. Australian Feminist Studies, 24(60), 233-249.
Braun, V. (2009b). Selling the perfect vulva. In C. Heyes and M. Jones (Eds.), Cosmetic surgery: A feminist anthology: Ashgate.
Braun, V., and Kitzinger, C. (2001). 'Snatch', 'hole', or 'honey pot'? Semantic categories and the problem of non-specificity in female genital slang. Journal of Sex Research, 38, 146-158.
Braun, V., and Wilkinson, S. (2001). Socio-cultural representations of the vagina. Journal of Reproductive and Infant Psychology, 19, 17-32.
Braun, V., and Wilkinson, S. (2003). Liability or asset? Women talk about the vagina. Psychology of Women Section Review, 5(2), 28-42.
Braun, V., and Wilkinson, S. (2005). Vagina equals woman? On genitals and gendered identity. Women's Studies International Forum, 28(6), 509-522.
Brooks, A. (2004). "Under the knife and proud of it:" An analysis of the normalization of cosmetic surgery. Critical Sociology, 30(2), 207-239.
Campbell, C. (2004). Health psychology and community action. In M. Murray (Ed.), Critical health psychology (pp. 203-221). Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.
Can surgery better sex? (2008). Company, July, 102-103.
Cartwright, R., and Cardozo, L. (2008). Cosmetic vulvovaginal surgery. Obstetrics, Gynaecology and Reproductive Medicine, 18(10), 285-286.
Choi, H. Y., and Kim, K. T. (2000). A new method for aesthetic reduction of the labia minora (the deepithelialized reduction labioplasty). Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery, 105(1), 419-422.
Cochrane, K. (2007). A choice too far. New Statesman, 136(4863), 30-31.
Conrad, P. (2007). The medicalization of society. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press.
Conroy, R. M. (2006). Female genital mutilation: Whose problem, whose solution? Britsh Medical Journal, 333, 106-107.
Corinna, H. (n.d.). Give'em Some Lip: Labia That Clearly Ain't Minor. Scarleteen. Retrieved from http://www.scarleteen.com/article/advice/giveem_some_lip_labia_that_clearly_aint_minor
Cosmetic labiaplasty: The great ethical debate. (2007, 26 March). Berman Sexual Health Retrieved 5 February, 2008, from http://www.bermansexualhealth.com/index2.php?option=com_contentanddo_pdf=1andid=99
Davis, K. (2003). Dubious equalities and embodied differences: Cultural studies on cosmetic surgery. Lanham, MD: Rowman and Littlefield.
Davis, S. W. (2002). Loose lips sink ships. Feminist Studies, 28(1), 7-35.
de Alencar Felicio, Y. (2007). Labial surgery. Aesthetic Surgery Journal, 27(3), 322-328.
Di Saia, J. P. (2008). An unusual staged labial rejuvenation. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 5, 1263-1267.
Doyle, J. (2007). Request for cosmetic genitoplasty: How should health care providers respond? [letter]. BMJ Rapid Responses [online].
Duke, L. L., and Kreshel, P. J. (1998). Negotiating femininity: Girls in early adolescence read teen magazines. Journal Of Communication Inquiry, 22(1), 48-71.
Elliott, A. (2008). Making the cut: How cosmetic surgery is transforming our lives. London: Reaktion Books.
Ellis, S. R. (2006). Subject positions in women's talk about female genitals. University of Saskatchewan, Saskatoon.
Ensler, E. (1998). The vagina monologues. New York: Villard.
Essén, B., and Johnsdotter, S. (2004). Female genital mutilation in the West: Traditional circumcision versus genital cosmetic surgery. Acta Obstetricia et Gynecologica Scandinavica, 83(7), 611-613.
Fausto-Sterling, A. (2000). Sexing the body: Gender politics and the construction of sexuality. New York: Basic Books.
Fitzpatrick, M. (2007). Brazilian bikini wax and the designer vagina. British Journal of General Practice, 57, 1005.
Fitzpatrick, L. (2008, November 19). Plastic surgery below the belt. Time. Available: http://www.time.com/time/health/article/0,8599,1859937,00.html
Fliegner, J. R. H. (1997). Vulval varicosities and labial reduction. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 37(1), 129-130.
Fraser, S. (2003). Cosmetic surgery, gender and culture. Houndmills, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.
Freudenberg, N. (2005). Public health advocacy to change corporate practices: Implications for health education practice and research. Health Education and Behavior, 32(3), 1-22.
Gill, R. (2008). Empowerment/sexism: Figuring female sexual agency in contemporary advertising. Feminism and Psychology, 18(1), 35-60.
Gilman, S. L. (1985). Difference and pathology: Stereotypes of sexuality, race and madness. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.
Gilman, S. L. (1998). Creating beauty to cure the soul. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
Gilman, S. L. (1999). Making the body beautiful: A cultural history of aesthetic surgery. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press.
Giraldo, F., González, C., and de Haro, F. (2004). Central wedge nymphectomy with a 90-degree Z-plasty for aesthetic reduction of the labia minora. Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery, 113(6), 1820-1825.
Goodman, M. P., Bachmann, G., Crista, J., Fourcroy, J. L., Goldstein, A., Goldstein, G., et al. (2007). Is elective vulvar plastic surgery ever warranted, and what screening should be conducted preoperatively? The Journal of Sexual Medicine, 4, 269-276.
Green, F. J. (2005). From clitoridectomies to 'designer vaginas': The medical construction of heteronormative female bodies and sexuality through female genital cutting. Sexualities, Evolution and Gender, 7(2), 153-187.
Grosz, E. (1994). Volatile bodies: Toward a corporeal feminism. Sydney: Allen and Unwin.
Häggström-Nordin, E., Sandberg, J., Hanson, U., and Tydén, T. (2006). 'It's everywhere!' Young Swedish people's thoughts and reflections about pornography. Scandinavian Journal of Caring Sciences, 20, 386-393.
Haiken, E. (1997). Venus envy: A history of cosmetic surgery. Baltimore, MD: The Johns Hopkins University Press.
Harris, D. L., and Carr, A. T. (2001). Prevalence of concern about physical appearance in the general population. British Journal of Plastic Surgery, 54(3), 223-226.
Hart, G., and Wellings, K. (2002). Sexual behaviour and its medicalisation: in sickness and health. British Medical Journal, 324, 896-900.
Hawkes, G. (1996). The sociology of sex and sexuality. Buckingham, UK: Open University Press.
Hegarty, P., and Chase, C. (2000). Intersex activism, feminism, and psychology: Opening a dialogue on theory, research and clinical practice. Feminism and Psychology, 10, 117-132.
Hicks, K. (1994 ). Surviving the Dalkon Shield IUD. New York: Teachers College Press.
Hodgkinson, D. J., and Hait, G. (1984). Aesthetic vaginal labioplasty. Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery, 74(3), 414-416.
James, S. M., and Robertson, C. C. (Eds.). (2002). Genital cutting and transnational sisterhood: Disputing U.S. polemics. Chicago: University of Illinois Press.
Jeffreys, S. (2005). Beauty and misogyny: Harmful cultural practices in the West. London: Routledge.
Kapsalis, T. (1997). Public privates: Performing gynecology from both ends of the speculum. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
Karkazis, K. A. (2008). Fixing sex: Intersex, medical authority, and lived experience. Durham, NC: Duke University Press.
Kaw, E. (1998). Medicalization of racial features: Asian-American women and cosmetic surgery. In R. Weitz (Ed.), The Politics of Women's Bodies (pp. 167-183). New York: Oxford University Press.
Kessler, S. J. and McKenna, W. (1978). Gender: An ethnomethodological approach. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Kilbourne, J. (1999). Can't buy my love: How advertising changes the way we think and feel. New York: Touchstone.
Kristeva, J. (1982). Powers of horror: An essay in abjection (L. S. Roudiez, Trans.). New York: Columbia University Press.
Landman, B. (2004). Struck twice. New York, (1 March). Retrieved from http://nymag.com/nymetro/news/trends/columns/cityside/n_9981/
Laws, S. (1987). Down there - Using speculums for self-examination. In S. O'Sullivan (Ed.), Women's health: A Spare Rib reader (pp. 8-14). London: Pandora.
Lee, P. A., Houk, C. P., Ahmed, S. F., Hughes, I. A., in collaboration with the participants in the International Consensus Conference on Intersex organized by the Lawson Wilkins Pediatric Endocrine, S., and the European Society for Paediatric, E. (2006). Consensus statement on management of intersex disorders. Pediatrics, 118(2), e488-500.
Liao, L. M., and Creighton, S. (2007). Requests for cosmetic genitoplasty: How should healthcare providers respond? British Medical Journal, 334, 1090-1092.
Lloyd, J., Crouch, N. S., Minto, C. L., Liao, L.-M., and Creighton, S. (2005). Female genital appearance: 'Normality' unfolds. British Journal of Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 112, 643-646.
Lomax, H., and Casey, N. (1998). Recording social life: Reflexivity and video methodology. Sociological Research Online, 3(2).
Maas, S. M., and Hage, J. J. (2000). Functional and aesthetic labia minora reduction. Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery, 105, 1453-1456.
Manderson, L. (2004). Local rites and body politics: Tensions between cultural diversity and human rights. International Feminist Journal of Politics, 6(2), 285-307.
Martin, E. (1987). The woman in the body: A cultural analysis of reproduction. Open University Press: Milton Keynes.
McLean, T. (2007). Research to reveal why women opt for permanent Brazilians, AAP Newswire Available from http://www.highbeam.com/doc/1P1-135992646.html
McNamara, K. R. (2006). Pretty woman: Genital plastic surgery and the production of the sexed female subject. GNovis, 7. Retrieved from http://www.gnovisjournal.org/journal/pretty-woman-genital-plastic-surgery-and-production-sexed-female-subject
Morgen, S. (2002 ). Into our own hands: The women’s health movement in the United States, 1969-1990. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Morrison, T. G., Bearden, A., Ellis, S. R., and Harriman, R. (2005). Correlates of genital perceptions among Canadian post-secondary students. Electronic Journal of Human Sexuality, 8, [online] accessed 1 March 2006 from http://www.ejhs.org/volume2008/GenitalPerceptions.htm.
Most women 'want plastic surgery'. (2001, August 8). Retrieved April 21, 2004, from http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/health/1480597.stm
Moynihan, R., and Cassels, A. (2005). Selling sickness: How drug companies are turning us all into patients. Sydney, Australia: Allen and Unwin.
Munhoz, A. M., Filassi, J. R., Ricci, M. D., Aldrighi, C., Correia, L., Aldrighi, J. M., et al. (2006). Aesthetic labia minora reduction with inferior wedge resection and superior pedicle flap reconstruction. Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery, 118, 1237-1247.
Murray, M. (Ed.). (2004). Critical health psychology. Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.
Murray, M., and Campbell, C. (2003). Living in a material world: Reflecting assumptions of health psychology. Journal of Health Psychology, 8, 231-236.
National Women’s Health Network. (2002 ). The truth about hormone replacement therapy. Roseville, CA: Prima Publishing.
Negrin, L. (2002). Cosmetic surgery and the eclipse of identity. Body and Society, 8(4), 21-42.
Njambi, W. N. (2004). Dualisms and female bodies in representations of African female circumcision: a feminist critique. Feminist Theory, 5(3), 281-303.
Olembo, A. (2007). Community partnerships to eradicate FGM in rural Somalia. Paper presented at the Global Health Conference, available: http://www.globalhealth.org/conference_2007/presentations/a_olembo.pdf.
Pardo, J., Solà, V., Ricci, P. A., and Guilloff, E. (2006). Laser labioplasty of labia minora. International Journal of Gynaecology and Obstetrics, 93, 38-43.
Pardo, J. S., Solà, V., Ricci, P. A., Guilloff, E. F., and Freundlich, O. (2006). Colpoperineoplasty in women with a sensation of a wide vagina. Acta Obstetrica et Gynecologica Scandinavica, 85, 1125-1127.
Parker, R. (2007). Sex smarts: All about VaginaPagina: Community offers images of 'real' vulvas, rather than of porn stars. The Penn. Retrieved from http://media.www.thepenn.org/media/storage/paper930/news/2007/02/23/Accent/Sex-Smarts.All.About.Vaginapagina.Community.Offers.Images.Of.real.Vulvas.Rather-2740098.shtml
Payer, L. (1992). Disease-mongers: How doctors, drug companies and insurers are making you feel sick. New York: Wiley.
Peixoto Labre, M. (2002). The Brazilian wax: New hairlessness norm for women? Journal of Communication Inquiry, 26(2), 113-132.
Pitts-Taylor, V. (2007). Surgery junkies: Wellness and pathology in cosmetic culture. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Pliskin, K. L. (1995). Vagina dentata revisited: Gender and asymptomatic shedding of genital herpes. Culture, Medicine, and Psychiatry, 19, 479-501.
Preminger, B. (2001). The "Jewish Nose" and plastic surgery: Origins and implications. Journal of the American Medical Association, 286(17), 2161.
Rahman, A., and Toubia, N. (2000). Female genital mutilation: A guide to laws and policies worldwide. London: Zed Books.
Redd, N. A. (2008). Body drama. New York: Gotham Books.
Reinholtz, R. K., and Muehlenhard, C. L. (1995). Genital perceptions and sexual activity in a college population. Journal of Sex Research, 32(2), 155-165.
Rodriguez, S. W. (2008). Rethinking the history of female circumcision and clitoridectomy: American medicine and female sexuality in the late nineteenth century. Journal Hist Med Allied Sci, 63(3), 323-347.
Rouzier, R., Louis-Sylvestre, C., Paniel, B. J., and Haddad, B. (2000). Hypertrophy of the labia minora: Experience with 163 reductions. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology, 182(1), 35-40.
Sarwer, D., Cash, T. F., Magee, L. B. A., Williams, E, Thompson, J. K. P. D., Roehrig, M., et al. (2005). Female college students and cosmetic surgery: An investigation of experiences, attitudes, and body image. Plastic and Reconstructive Surgery, 115(3), 931-938.
Sheehan, E. A. (1997). Victorian clitoridectomy: Isaac Baker Brown and his harmless operative procedure. In R. N. Lancaster and M. di Leonardo (Eds.), The gender/sexuality reader: Culture, history, political economy (pp. 325-334). New York: Routledge.
Singer, N. (2008). A spa for those women concerned about ‘pelvic fitness’. The New York Times, July 3. Retrieved from http://www.nytimes.com/2008/07/03/fashion/03SkinOne.html
Somerville, S. (1997). Scientific racism and the invention of the homosexual body. In R. N. Lancaster and M. di Leonardo (Eds.), The gender/sexuality reader: Culture, history, political economy (pp. 37-52). New York: Routledge.
Stainton Rogers, W. (1996). Critical approaches to health psychology. Journal of Health Psychology, 1, 65-77.
Stam, H. J. (2004). A sound mind in a sound body: A critical historical analysis of health psychology. In M. Murray (Ed.), Critical health psychology (pp. 15-30). Basingstoke, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.
Sullivan, D. A. (2001). Cosmetic surgery: The cutting edge of commercial medicine in America. New Brunswick, NJ: Rutgers University Press.
Sullivan, N. (2007). "The Price to Pay for our Common Good": Genital modification and the somatechnologies of cultural (in)difference. Social Semiotics, 17, 395-409.
Terry, J. (1990). Lesbians under the medical gaze: Scientists search for remarkable differences. Journal of Sex Research, 27(3), 317-339.
Terry, J. (1995). Anxious slippages between 'us' and 'them': A brief history of the scientific search for homosexual bodies. In J. Terry and J. Urla (Eds.), Deviant bodies: Critical perspectives on difference in science and popular culture (pp. 129-169). Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press.
The American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists. (2006). ACOG Committee Opinion No. 341: Ethical ways for physicians to market a practice. Obstetrics and Gynecology, 108(1), 239-242.
The American College of Obstetricians and Gynecologists. (2007). Vaginal ‘rejuvenation’ and cosmetic vaginal procedures (Committee Opinion Piece No. 378). Obstetrics and Gynecology, 110, 737-738.
The Royal Australian and New Zealand College of Obstetricians and Gynaecologists. (2008). Vaginal 'rejuvenation' and cosmetic vaginal procedures, New College Statement C-Gyn 24. Melbourne: The Royal Australian and New Zealand College of Obstetricians and Gynaecologists.
Tiefer, L. (2006). Female sexual dysfunction: A case study of disease mongering and activist resistance. PloS Medicine, 3, 436-439.
Tiefer, L. (2008a). Female cosmetic genital surgery: Freakish or inevitable? Analysis from medical marketing, bioethics, and feminist theory. Feminism and Psychology, 18(4), 466-479.
Tiefer, L. (2008b). The New View in activism and academics 10 years on. Feminism and Psychology, 18(4), 451-456.
Tilley, E. (2007, 10 May). Wax lyrical. The Courier Mail Retrieved 15 May, 2007.
Tracy, E. E. (2007). Elective vulvoplasty: A bandage that might hurt. Obstetrics and Gynaecology, 109, 1179-1180.
Trager, J. D. K. (2006). Pubic hair remova l-- Pearls and pitfalls. Journal of Pediatric and Adolescent Gynecology, 19(2), 117-123.

Ussher, J. (2006). Managing the monstrous feminine: Regulating the reproductive body. London: Routledge.

--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

Footnotes:

[1] Different polls give different results. Of the 655 respondents to the current thefrisky.com poll on the same topic, 17% indicated they would consider labiaplasty, and 13% that they might (response categories were worded somewhat differently, see http://www.thefrisky.com/site/post/246-poll-would-you-ever-snip-down-below). Less enthusiasm was shown by 85% of respondents in Company Magazine's (UK) “biggest ever surgery survey” who reported that they would “never consider having surgery done down there” ("Can surgery better sex?," 2008, p. 102) - although 15% apparently would, and number of respondents was not indicated. Sound empirical research on interest and uptake is lacking.

[2] In a different way, it is also profoundly commercial, and tied into economic factors and imperatives as well (Elliott, 2008; Haiken, 1997; Sullivan, 2001), which we should not forget in our analyses, and deeply cultural.

[3] Critics (e.g., Allotey, Manderson, and Grover, 2001; Conroy, 2006; Essén and Johnsdotter, 2004; Manderson, 2004; Sullivan, 2007) have also raised questions about the practical, moral, ethical and legal aspects of the different ways western and non-western genital 'cuttings' are treated in the west, such that FGCS is not subject to legal contestations, although laws against 'female genital mutilation' technically prohibit some forms of labiaplasty, for instance (Sullivan, 2007).

[4] There is much controversy over terminology in this area, not least because the practices are diverse. This has seen the emergence of less 'judgmental' terms like 'female genital cutting,' which not all agree with. Our views on these issues are not uniform or singular. We use the term 'FGM' in this paper, but in scare quotes, although we recognize that some readers will find it problematic.

[5] We must acknowledge this account around FGM activism as singular and one which focuses on the positives of international efforts against it. However, much controversy exists in this area, and activism against FGM (by western feminists/organisations) has been subject to substantial critique, and there are ongoing debates about who should be involved in moves to eradicate FGM practices, and how these might be put into practice. Both insiders (women from countries where various 'traditional' forms of 'cutting' are practiced) and outsiders (women from countries where they are not) present a wide range of differing viewpoints in these debates. Critics have suggested that western activism continues long histories of cultural imperialism and colonisation, obliterates the complexities of meaning and practices around genital cutting, and situates African women as 'uncivilised' within a matrix of cultural influence that can only be challenged by outside perspectives (e.g., see Abusharaf, 2001; James and Robertson, 2002; Njambi, 2004).

[6] The Consensus Statement is not without its critics, not least in relation to the shift in terminology, from “intersex” to “disorders of sexual development” (e.g., http://aiclegal.org/yabb/YaBB.pl?num=1218845029/4; http://intersexpride.blogspot.com/)

[7] Where critical analysts identify that disease-mongering involves the creation of new 'diseases' to fit treatments (Moynihan and Cassels, 2005), those within the pharmaceutical industry refer more positively to 'condition branding' (Angelmar, Angelmar, and Kane, 2007).


--------------------------------------------------------------------------------

Biographical notes:


Virginia Braun is a Senior Lecturer in Psychology at The University of Auckland. Her research is located within feminist and critical psychology, and focuses on topics related to sex, sexual health, and female genital cosmetic surgery. She is Co-Editor, with Nicola Gavey (The University of Auckland), of the journal Feminism & Psychology.

Leonore Tiefer is a clinical psychologist in the Psychiatry Department at the NYU School of Medicine. She is author, educator, researcher, therapist and activist in sexology and has keynoted conferences around the world. In 2000 Dr. Tiefer initiated the Campaign for a New View of Women's Sexual Problems [http://newviewcampaign.org] to challenge the new medicalization and Big Pharma trends. She is the author of Sex Is Not a Natural Act (2nd edition, Westview, 2004) and other works.

Tuesday, 4 January 2011

The Hyper-Manipulated Self

The War on Reason
by Brian C. Taylor

A philosophical examination of the intentional aspects of the events that occurred on Manhattan Island on September 11th, 2001 and some key sociological byproducts.


This is not an essay about 9/11 as a conspiracy theory, it is about the dichotomy of opinion that is created by the need for belief in either the official story or the various theories that are available. This is an essay about hypermanipulation.

This is an edited version of a chapter for my forthcoming book: Anti-Social Engineering the Hyper Manipulated Self due 2011.

Comments are welcome.

~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~~


No matter how you reduce it, the events that took place on Manhattan Island, September 11, 2001 were a direct result of the intentions of the American government. How far you go into conspiracy theory is up to you. I've heard them all, but I'm not here to subscribe to any of them. You know me, you know our goals here. I'm going to go with what I have: things that we agree are indisputable facts and logic. It is in the interest of full disclosure that I present all these theories of what happened that day. It is a necessary part of my overall argument that the dichotomy of opinion be illustrated. I mean no disrespect to anyone, in fact, in this story, I see everyone as victim. It is not only their arguments we must think about, we must also recognize how it's all argument and what that means.

So, what can we say for certain about what happened that day? It's hard to imagine anyone thinking that “nothing” happened that day and I'm certainly unaware of any such theory coming forward. Everyone agrees that “something” happened that day. Other things we have to believe happened, but a few things are given. There used to be three more towers at the World Trade Center complex. Those towers are not there because they were destroyed on that day. Surprisingly, this is where what we actually know and can all agree on as happening, ends. No matter how you look at the rest of the story, you are going to be able to find more than one person who thinks differently. To some, this may be of no consequence. Those of us with paradigm paralysis cannot change our minds, those of us who won't even hear such nonsense, won't change our minds. You and I, however, understand something of how the mind works, how the self is shaped. You and I are beginning to understand how, at times, we can't even trust ourselves. We know what the difference between knowing and believing is. We can see the value in the big picture. We can decide for ourselves and, more importantly, we can not decide if we so choose. As I've stated, everyone is entitled to believe whatever nonsense they like. Some of us are also able to explain why we believe things without saying, “I just do.”

First, I will explain the official account of the day, as briefly as I can before I open a can of worms. As usual, we seek the who, what, where, when, how and why of the events that lead to the destruction that took place at the World Trade Center on September eleventh. (We will also discuss the other events of the day.) We all agree that, in the end, we had three towers reduced to dust, thousands of people dead and missing. How did the towers collapse and what was the cause? The official, accepted version is actually quite simple: Two large commercial airliners crashed, one each in each of the towers. The planes were laden with fuel and the subsequent explosion and fire weakened structural supports over the nearly two hours before the collapse. Finally, once the supports let go, the beams holding up the floor collapsed to the floor below, which was also weakened. Thus, the floor below collapsed, then the floor below, until such momentum and weight was achieved that the towers disintegrated in a “pancake” effect, floor to floor at nearly free fall speed. First one tower collapsed, then the other. Then, a few hours later, the third tower, the building known as WTC 7, collapsed. (We will discuss this event separately.)

The official explanation of how the aircraft were being used is also very clear cut. Members of Al Queda, controlled and funded by known terrorist kingpin Osama bin Laden, wished to wage a holy war against America. The hijackers used boxcutters and brute force to gain entry into the cockpits of these airplanes with the intention of crashing them into specific targets in the US. Which, for all but one of the four planes, they did. The fourth crashed in a field in Pennsylvania. Officials later stated they became aware of the boxcutter methodology via passenger cell phone calls to loved ones, the police, the FBI, etc. All of the men boarded the plane as ticketed passengers. All of them were Muslim. Some of the men were known to be training as pilots in the US. The highjackers, as they became known, were from different middle eastern countries. Pakistan, Sudan, most of them were from Saudi Arabia. Some of them, later identified, were known “muslim extremists.” The Pentagon was hit by the third highjacked airliner that came in at a ground level trajectory. There is no useable video footage of this event but there are still photos of the aftermath.


So we've covered everything but the "official" why."Which is the most difficult to cover and the point at which we have no choice but to accept someone's opinion. Even if we believe the tape of Osama bin Laden taking responsibility for the events and saying that he did so in retaliation for the US supporting Israel and occupying Muslim lands, it is still his version, which differs than the American version of “Muslim extremism hates everything about America, especially freedom.” Now, looking back on this statement, it seems almost farcical, yet we all heard it countless times.

Now, before we open that can of worms, peer inside and see what kind of weirdness is wriggling around, let's examine just a few more accepted truisms about the intentional background of the overall story. This will entail a little history. On December 24, 1979, Russia sent troops in to assist the government of Afghanistan in an ongoing civil war against the Mujahideen. The Mujahideen were, and are still, essentially Muslim freedom fighters. Afghanistan's government, The Democratic Republic of Afghanistan was Communist and wished to advance the government, into a more modern, Eastern place. This type of coup was not at all a new thing for the area, even a cursory glance at Afghanistan's history will prove to you that it is, and always has been, an area of upheaval, perhaps become of it's central location in what we call the middle east. This time however, the government asked the Soviets for assistance and got it. Of course America didn't like this, particularly after the Shah they place in Iran was ousted and the American ambassador was kidnapped, then executed. (And let's not forget, the cold war is still in effect.) America, and some of her allies, via the CIA funnelled arms and supplies into Afghanistan, often through Pakistan, to support the Mujahideen in their “patriotic struggle.” This literally put bin Laden and his resources in the loop. He didn't need to “be on the payroll” but for all intents and purposes, he was. He was assisted in every way imaginable, short of putting American troops on the ground there, in any form other than training or advisory.

Then, in the spring of 1988, Russia, having spent billions of dollars and lost countless young men, she gave up and began removing her troops. Some people call the Russia Afghanistan conflict “Russia's Vietnam” because it was unpopular, seemingly unwinnable and promising little reward in the first place, (or so it seemed to the populace.) With Russia retreating, the Mujahideen claimed Afghanistan as victors, which I guess they were. The price they paid however, was continued chaos. Afghanistan has always been a tribal area or tribal people. (One little ten year war, in the grand scheme of eons of tradition of a proud people(s) is unlikely to change things for very long.) Until finally, the Taliban takes control of the government in 1996. The Taliban are aptly named, the word means “student,” and the Taliban, above all honours it's interpretation of the faith. What George Bush sees as Muslim extremism, to the Taliban, is the will of God. This is serious business and a great many people willingly take part in this seriousness.

The west used the Mujahideen for it's own goals and then abandoned them, albeit with fanfare and respect, to their own devices. Reagan recognized them as “freedom fighters” (which is a direct translation) as did James Bond and Rambo. While being applauded and immortalized in film nearing the end of the cold war, the CIA was quietly jotting down the names of the leaders, Liutenants and key players in the Mujahideen. These people, again, the more extreme, more violent members, became known as “the foundation” which translates into "Al Queda." (Foundation as in "base" a structure to be built upon.) Al Queda continues to fight for that which they consider holy.

Now that we have established, albeit curtly, the basic official story including the main relevant aspects, you may have noticed that it, in someways, is contradictory, or at least shows discrepancies. Before we look at arguments against the official story as I've relayed it, let's open the flood gates of conspiracy and see what comes pouring in. Remember that we have no interest in wantonly suppressing opinions, we don't even have to decide if these seem reasonable or not, at this point. Let's just hear some other peoples original ideas, then we'll talk about them. Because there are so many of them, I will list them in point form as briefly as possible, arranged in topics of my choosing:

-Airplane issues: There are several theories around the planes, some say they could fly as slow as they appear to be doing on film, some think they were remote controlled, some think they were not passenger aircraft at all, but were made up to look like they were, this is attached to the theory that there were no passengers, some think the airplanes had tanks attached to the underside for a larger explosion, some think there were no planes at all, they were drawn in, on the spot, in one of the largest, live, special effects ever to take place. (We will discuss this theory separately.) Finally, some argue that the cell phone calls people got couldn't have taken place as the technology wasn't on-board commercial airliners in 2001.

-Pilot issues: As I mentioned, some of the terrorists that took control of these airplanes were taking courses at American flight schools. None of those that were had impressive skills. Some of the instructors we're quoted expressing their dismay that these men could fly at all, never mind the rather difficult flying it took to successfully hit three of four targets in first attempts at flying a jumbo jet. This is distinct enough to be listed separately from Airplane issues.

-Architectural issues: There is currently a very large and growing group called “Architects for truth” that claims it is and was scientifically impossible for a building to be destroyed as it was, by fire. Therefore, they argue, these buildings were a controlled demolition.

-Controlled Demolition argument: Simply put, the argument is “these buildings were pre-rigged to be destroyed. The airplanes were subterfuge that was necessary or it would be too obvious that this was a false flag event. (How could you secretly rig three giant buildings to be demolished?) Evidence to support this theory are the several large explosions before the planes hit, (testimonial of employees of WTC,) the damage to the WTC1 lobby, (testimonial of firefighters,) explosions visible from the exterior as the towers collapsed, (testimonial of firefighters and other eyewitnesses,) and finally, after the collapse it was discovered that the remaining beams, jutting out from the rubble were sheared in perfect diagonal lines, in exactly the fashion one would use in a controlled demolition.

-False Flag argument: Regardless of how it came to be, this argument claims that it happened because it was supposed to. Perhaps some faction of the American government participated in the act, perhaps they just let it happen. Either way, it was a “business decision to keep the American military industrial complex going.”

-American complicity argument: see final image. They knew it was coming.”

-American duplicity argument: “They wanted it all along.” The Project for a New American Century was a politically based think tank established in the 1980's by Neoconservatives William Kristol and Robert Kagan. In 1997 they published a ninety page document entitled, “Rebuilding America's Defences.” I quote now from part five: “Further, the process of transformation, even if it brings revolutionary change, is likely to be a long one, absent some catastrophic and catalyzing event––like a new Pearl Harbor.” This is a very famous sentence referring to the need for something to “call people to arms.” Their explicit goal in this document was to recommend war in the middle east to control oil reserves, continue to expand military dominance and keep the economy of war moving forward. Somehow, they got what they wanted.

-The Strange Coincidences argument: This truly is a strange and publicly documented series of events. On September 11th 2001, the US military, NORAD, the National Reconnaissance Office, private and public law enforcement and related organizations were taking part in exercises that simulated things like, Russian/Nato wargames in the arctic, bioterrorism attacks, hijacked planes being flown into buildings. The purpose of this exercise was to test the preparedness of the American defence system in case of a terrorist attack on US soil. If you are confused, I'll reiterate. The government agencies involved in dealing with a situation like the one that occurred on September 11th, were doing a drill on September 11th that exactly mirrored the actual events taking place, in real time. (This is not actually a conspiracy theory. This was actually taking place. It becomes a conspiracy theory only through the reasonable expectation that by this happening, fighter planes were out of the area, confusion was created, delays occurred. This argument gets even spookier when we later discover that the exact same exercises were being conducted in the exact same fashion by British counterparts on the day of their terrorist attacks in London on July 7th, 2007.

-Discrepancies and “faked evidence:” Here the argument is, “the logic of their explanations is flawed.” For instance, if the two largest towers collapsed to dust, by way of the “fire caused a pancake collapse” theory, why did tower seven collapse when it suffered relatively minor fire damage and had a completely different construction style? Furthermore, the owner of the entire World Trade Center complex Larry Silverstein is on record as saying, “after we saw the damage caused by the other two towers collapsing, we made the decision that the best thing to do (with tower 7) would be to pull it.” He later tried to renounce this statement, saying that he never intended “pull it” to mean bring the tower down with explosives, he meant “stop fighting the fire and let it go down,” as in “pull the firefighters out.” Still, when you watch the footage of this collapse, it is picture perfect. So, we are back to our "controlled demolition argument."


There were other strange things about this aspect of the story. For instance, some of the alleged highjackers were never onboard the plane. Some of them, upon hearing they were dead and how, called American press agencies to denounce the news, obviously very alive and having nothing to do with any of these events. Other things were introduced as evidence that seemed illogical or even impossible. There is the case of a terrorist's passport being found at ground zero, completely unharmed. (The plane was completely destroyed in a fire so hot that it brought down a skyscraper, which has never happened before, yet a paper passport survived to be found on the ground?) A few blocks away from the WTC complex a jet engine was found that was claimed to be from the aircraft used. It was the wrong type of engine and in pristine condition. Then there is the case of confiscated video footage being returned edited. There were approximately 175 video cameras recording what happened that day. Of course many of them belonged to television stations but a number of them were just caught by citizens of New York and tourists. Of these, the majority were confiscated by the FBI in what could be argued to be a reasonable search for evidence. However, some of these tapes came back with footage missing, or they were copies of the tapes, not the originals. So the camera might pan away or cut away at key points, to miss the face of a passer-by, or even the critical moment of impact. Finally, there is the official footage which also seems edited, such as the Pentagon video that was released with “missing frames” at the critical moment. The Pentagon says the video is unedited and the plane was moving too fast for the video to catch it. I don’t know enough about video or physics to answer this question myself. Other, official news agency footage also seems to “cut away” at critical moments. In this category, there is a lot of material to absorb, some of which ties into the...


No Airplane theory: this theory is most convincingly argued in a film called “September Clues.” Basically, this extremely complicated argument boils down to the following: There never were any planes, (we don't know what happened to the passengers.) The explosions were caused by missiles being fired from the ground or boats nearby. Then “black ops” specialists, cut into the feeds of the news agencies satellites, in a seven second delay, to superimpose pre-prepared “airplanes” into the pictures that then were “sent out” as live footage. The film makers go into great detail answering all the questions you're currently coming up with, as I said, this is probably the most complicated of the conspiracy theories. This theory may have stemmed from contradictory eyewitness accounts, some saw a small place, a silver plane, a white plane, a plane with no windows, a plane that looked like a missile, a missile and finally nothing, the building just exploded in the shape of a wingspan. The filmmakers also argue that the frame by frame analysis shows a perfectly shaped “nose” of the second plane exiting the other side and retracting. They argue that this was a mistake by the black op fakers.


Advanced Secret Weaponry theory: This argument states that, again, this event was a pre-planned, controlled demolition, but there is also evidence of Scalar weaponry being used and Thermite. Scalar weapons are a (so far,) theoretical “ray gun” like weapon that can do many magical things. It is said the use of scalar weaponry is evidenced by black helicopters circling the towers, emitting very bright lights and vehicles at ground zero being completely unharmed, other than they are upside down. (The list of things that it is said a scalar ray can do is long, but includes: anti-gravity, molecular disruption, super heat.) Thermite, a very real entity, it is an extremely hot chemical reaction used to cut, weld or otherwise melt metals. It is claimed it is visible pouring out of the still standing towers and then, after the collapses, rendering pools of remarkably hot molten metal that stayed hot for weeks. Firefighters, attempting to search for survivors were sometimes turned away from these pools, too hot to approach.

“Too weird for this world” theories: Some people think that the events that took place on September 11th were some kind of Illuminati, satanist, alien agenda human sacrifice. Probably most famously put forward by David Icke, the Bush family, descended from Nero (which is true,) is part of the same bloodline as Queen Elizabeth, (and all the Windsor's, also true,) but that they are actually shape-shifting reptilian aliens hellbent on enslaving mankind as a food source, (probably not true.) Although the Bush family is involved in some weirdness, in the shape of the controversial membership to organizations such as the Skull and Bones, or the Bilderbergers, or taking part in the secret meetings at Bohemian Grove, people can only speculate on what this actually means, if anything. One weird aspect that I find spooky because you can see it with your own eyes and although it's probably a coincidence, but what a coincidence, is the Cash Talisman theory. This is best explained in pictures, featured below. What is argued is that, by handling this money, day in day out, for years, we subconsciously, (cosmically?) made these events happen through our unconscious intentions. In the frames, by folding the American dollar bills of different denominations we can see a distinct series of events unfold on 5, 10, 20, 50 and 100 dollar bills, viewed through the Washington arch near New York University, if folded "correctly"


I may have missed certain theories and I've certainly not gone into any great detail on any of them. Before we start examining these theories, let's take a moment to review what were the results of the events that took place on September 11th. I'm sure that everyone can agree on this: America and it's allies moved into Afghanistan and Iraq, yet not Pakistan, Sudan, Saudi Arabia. We were told, adamantly that there were “weapons of mass destruction” in Iraq that needed to be found and that Iraq was harbouring terrorists. In fact, we should now be able to quite easily recognize the obvious social engineering of the news footage of politicians at the time, WMD and the need to engage was repeated, ad nauseum, until we found ourselves convinced. Plus, let's not forget, at this point in time, America is still reeling in sadness and anger. America wanted revenge, it didn't take much to convince the public that revenge was what was right.

However, what do we really have here, in this situation. We have one side, a designed subscribing to Islamic extremism and the teachings of a Philosopher named Sayyid Qutb which is excellently described in the BBC documentary series, The Power of Nightmares. On the other side we have a designed subscribing to Christian extremism and the teachings of a Philosopher named Leo Strauss. Perhaps you are thinking to yourself, “Yes, but we didn't kill thousands of innocent Muslims in a terrorist attack.” If you are thinking this, please put your head down and take a nap, you are way too ignorant to continue this conversation. We have, historically and easily, out ranked the killing by a margin of one hundred to one. One cannot make the argument that “this type of killing” is okay and “that type of killing” is not. One cannot say, “We are right and they are wrong,” with any kind of accuracy. The terrorist's don't have a trillion dollar budget, they can only fight their war as they can. We have a ridiculous advantage over them, technologically, militarily and monetarily. We have no advantage over them morally, despite what we believe. This is a perfectly fair fight, because war isn't supposed to be fair. The difference we should note is, although we say we are fighting a just war for freedom, or to spread democracy, they are fighting a war to spread the right to live as they please, (is that not freedom?) They are not trying to spread anything, they would rather we left them alone. I'm not siding with terrorist's, I'm also not siding with anyone, I’m a philosopher. These latter points all but fall away when I tell you that we are only here to consider whether or not what I’ve stated is true, not whether it is right or wrong. We seek social engineering, we have it as “designed subscribing.”

Let's now look at reasonable problems we can find, both in the official version of events and the varied conspiracy theories. As we established at the beginning of this chapter, we actually know only two things: those building are gone and something happened that made them “go away.” So what should we think about the details? Who should we agree with? What should we believe? Firstly and probably most importantly, let's realize that the existence of this question, in and of itself, is the problem. Why should we have to believe anybody? How come we can't know what happened that day? The answer is, of course, that the pieces of the puzzle don't all fit together. As time moves forward, the one side, let's call them Conspiracy Theorists, pull further and further away from the other side, let's call them “Official Believers.” A puzzle with missing or incorrect pieces doesn't make a pretty picture, in fact, in makes nothing. So, let's start again in simple question and answer form, the answers will come from me, reasonably and honestly. These are my opinions on the matter.


Q: Did airplanes crash into the towers?

A: Yes, I think so. All those passengers are still missing. I think it's reasonable to expect that if it was part of “the charade,” at least one of them would turn up and say, “I'm still alive!” in much the same way that the terrorists who weren't involved did.

Q: Did the airplanes cause the towers' collapse?

A: I can't answer that, but I seriously doubt it. I'm convinced that there was at least a secondary method to cause the collapse as we know it happened. Whether it was traditional explosives, scalar weapons or thermite, whether it was Al Queda or American “black ops” I don't know. However, I side with the hundreds (is it thousands by now?) of architects who claim the collapse that happened that day was unprecedented at the least and impossible at the most. I'm also of the opinion that the collapse of building 7, along with Larry Silversteens instruction to “pull it,” is highly suspect. Building 7 is a smoking gun. There was no reason to expect it to collapse. If they “pulled it” it had to be rigged. If it was rigged, it was done weeks in advance. If it was done weeks in advance, it is, by definition, a conspiracy. Pools of thermite were present, this is documented. Thermite does not occur naturally. Somebody put it there. Thermite is a smoking gun. If someone put thermite there, it is, by definition, a conspiracy. I also think we should call into question the numerous eyewitness (and earwitness, if I may,) reports of “explosions” happening before, during and after the buildings smouldered, but hadn't yet collapsed. Also, the apparent explosions visible as the buildings collapsed. Structural explosions are strategic explosions, if they were happening, it is, by definition a conspiracy.


Q: How could they rig three huge buildings to be demolished, secretly?

A: I don't know. I do know that these buildings were very busy places and it wouldn't be uncommon for “construction workers” to be working there. Also, some WTC employees stated that there was “a lot of commotion, banging and screeching” heard from various floors, known to be vacant, in the weeks leading up to September 11th. All I know is this, steel framed buildings can burn and burn and burn, for weeks, without collapsing. Steel framed buildings do not collapse accidentally. If this type of building was going to do anything from the plane hitting it, (which, by the way, it was designed to withstand,) it would have a section that “fell over.” This is best illustrated when we watch the footage of the towers collapsing in slow motion. When the collapse begins, you can see how the top section, above the impact zone, starts to fall over sideways. Then, for some reason, dissolves upward in the same fashion as the rest of the building dissolves downward. This defies all logic and cannot be explained, by anyone, to my satisfaction. How is this even possible?

Q: Are you saying that America killed thousands of it's own people so they could advance an agenda?

A: No. I'm saying someone killed thousands of people in a way that isn't being fully or truthfully explained to us and it advanced a stated American political agenda. Moreover, I feel the results speak for themselves. I think it certainly would be easier for America to achieve what happened that day, than say, Al Queda. I'm of the opinion that there is a rogue element in the corridors of power in Washington. Even if Americans did this, it doesn't mean “America” did it. Unfortunately, for everyone, the world does not make this distinction. Obviously, we don’t wish to lump anything into generality.

Q: Complicity or Duplicity?

A: Probably both. We know Dick Cheney continuously refused requests to scramble fighter jets. We know George Bush was reading “My pet goat.” We know there were exercises taking place that mimicked the actual events and this caused confusion. We know that reports of imminent attacks were ignored. If all of these things are just the byproducts of the rampant ineptitude of modernity, (which isn't too much of a stretch,) it was the hallmark of all imperfect timing. However, things like the Project for a New American Century's report “Rebuilding America's Defences,” are a little too perfect a coincidence for my taste. Let's not forget, history is rife with false flags that various governments now admit to. Will someone, someday admit to this?

Q: Do you think Bush is a reptilian shape-shifter that wants to enslave the human race as a food source?

A: No. In my opinion, there are only two motivators for these very human people, money and power.

Q: Are there things that we can find out for certain?

A: Yes. We should be able to determine several things, if we are allowed to. It should be fairly easy to prove that these buildings were demolished by examining the left-over steel from the towers. (That was removed immediately and destroyed or recycled, or so we’re told.) Nevertheless, we should be able to do model experiments to test the “structural fire leads to collapse” hypothesis. We should be able to find out if the phone calls that took place onboard the flights were even possible at the time. We should be able to answer the question of whether or not the planes were capable of doing the flying as depicted in the films. I’m sure there are many other things I’m not thinking of.

Finally, what does it mean? We know where we are now, nearly ten years later, fighting two multinational wars on nearly invisible fronts. Do we know why? Not really. We can only take sides. So what can be said about who we are now, compared to who we were then, are we more or less ourselves?

The events that happened on Manhattan Island on September 11th, 2001 created the dichotomy that pushes our society over into hyper reality. After the dust had settled and the War on Terror begun, you now have a choice to make, go to sleep and have opinion roll over you or go insane and let opinion roll over you. Either you buy the whole picture or you buy none of it. If you buy the whole picture you're denying too much evidence to be correct. If you believe that any part of it is fishy then the whole deck of cards comes down. If you believe it's fishy, you have little evidence to prove your case, thus people think you're a crazy person. I'm not a crazy person, yet I think something is fishy. Where do I fit in? It seems the answer is nowhere. The Hyper-Manipulated Self is awash in a sea of opinion and you must choose. I'm asking you to think about the choice you are making.

Perhaps you've noticed the expanding divide that has grown in America. Be it between the faithful and the atheists, republicans versus democrats, or conservatives fighting liberals, the gap is getting wider, more pronounced, louder and more violent. Examine the health care debate of 2009-2010, with people showing up at “townhall” meetings brandishing machine guns. Consider those who argue that socialized medicine is communism. Listen to the argument on one side that says, “If we get national health care, next you'll be living in a concentration camp,” and on the other side holds up a sign that reads, “Get a brain, morans!” How could any rational thinking person pick either of these sides? We can't. It's stupid to do so. Yet, here we are, we've been forced into this dichotomy. The middle is falling out of everything. With no middle there is no balance. With no balance there are only extremes. Having to choose an extreme is not a solution, unless your goal is chaos.

So what is the goal? I don't mean the small, step-goals such as “think this about that,” these things, at this point, should be obvious, I mean the overall goal. I think the goal is exemplified to us in the results of the actions who give us these intentions. At the onset of this chapter I made the statement, No matter how you reduce it, the events that took place on Manhattan Island, September 11, 2001 were a direct result of the intentions of the American government. I base this statement on the complicity, duplicity and desires of the powers that be. The government may not have had an active role in the events that lead to the results they obviously wanted, but this only means they were lacking intention in action. If the prior intention of a small group leads to the action of others, we simply have collective intentionality. I'm not suggesting that the twin towers talisman worked, but rather that, it is no mistake that we find ourselves in the situation we are. It was socially engineered.

It is important that we recognize that this intention continues on in society, long after the engineers who drafted it and the politicians who enacted it are gone. We are not rid of a philosophy so easily, particularly when we become so invested in it.

They knew it was coming...




Project Syndicate: A World of Ideas


CAMBRIDGE – In a time of tight budgets and financial crisis, politicians nowadays look to economic growth as the centerpiece of their domestic policy programs. Gross domestic product is taken to be the leading indicator of national well-being. But, as we look ahead to 2011 and beyond, we should ask ourselves: is it really wise to accord such importance to growth?

Granted, many studies have confirmed that wealthier nations tend to be happier than poor ones, and that rich people are generally more satisfied than their less affluent fellow citizens. Yet other findings from several relatively well-to-do countries, such as South Korea and the United States, suggest that people there are essentially no happier today than they were 50 years ago, despite a doubling or quadrupling of average per capita income.

Moreover, in a recent Canadian study, the happiest people turned out to reside in the poorest provinces, such as Newfoundland and Nova Scotia, while citizens in the richest provinces, notably Ontario and British Columbia, were among the least happy. Since happiness is ultimately what people want the most, while wealth is only a means to that end, the primacy now accorded to economic growth would appear to be a mistake.

What seems clear from such research is that people do quite poorly at predicting what will make them happy or sad. They focus too much on their initial responses to changes in their lives and overlook how quickly the pleasure of a new car, a pay increase, or a move to sunnier climes will fade, leaving them no happier than before. It is hazardous, therefore, for politicians simply to rely on opinion polls and focus groups to discover what will truly enhance people’s happiness.

In the findings to date, however, two conclusions have emerged that seem especially useful for policymakers to ponder. First, most of the things that do bring enduring satisfaction for individuals are also good for other people – strong marriages and close relationships of all kinds, helping others, engaging in civic affairs, and effective, honest, democratic government. Thus, policies that promote individual well-being tend to benefit society as well.

Second, experiences that bring lasting pleasure or unhappiness do not always command a high priority in government circles. For example, three medical afflictions that create especially acute and enduring distress – clinical depression, chronic pain, and sleep disorders – are all conditions that can often be treated successfully, to the vast relief of sufferers. But such people are frequently underserved by health-care systems.

The natural response to all this is to ask whether happiness research is really reliable enough to be used by policymakers. Researchers have paid close attention to this issue, and, after much testing, have found that the answers people give to questions about their well-being seem to correspond fairly well to more objective evidence.

People who claim to be happy tend to live longer, commit suicide and abuse drugs and alcohol less often, get promoted more frequently by their employers, and enjoy more good friends and lasting marriages. Their assessments of their own well-being also align quite closely with the opinions of friends and family members.

So, overall, statistics about happiness seem to be as accurate as many of the statistics regularly used by politicians, such as public-opinion polls, poverty rates, or, for that matter, GDP growth – all of which are riddled with imperfections.

Of course, happiness research is still new. Many questions remain unexplored, some studies lack sufficient confirmatory evidence, and still others, like those involving the effects of economic growth, have yielded conflicting results.

Thus, it would be premature to base bold new policies on happiness research alone, or to follow the example of tiny Bhutan by adopting Gross National Happiness as the nation’s principal goal. Yet the findings may be useful to lawmakers even today – for example, in assigning priorities among several plausible initiatives, or in identifying new possibilities for policy interventions that deserve further study.

At the very least, governments should follow Great Britain and France and consider publishing regular statistics on trends in the well-being of their citizens. Such findings will surely stimulate useful public discussion while yielding valuable data for investigators to use.

Beyond that, who knows? Further research will doubtless provide more detailed and reliable information about the kinds of policies that add to people’s happiness. Someday, perhaps, public officials may even use the research to inform their decisions. After all, what could matter more to their constituents than happiness? In a democracy, at least, that should surely count for something.

Derek Bok, President of Harvard University from 1971-1991 and from 2006-2007, is the author of The Politics of Happiness (Princeton University Press, 2010).

Copyright: Project Syndicate/Institute for Human Sciences, 2011.
http://www.project-syndicate.org/



Monday, 3 January 2011

SEXIS What you want to be


by A Modern Love Muse


Science offers clues, but doesn’t have the answers—maybe because “desire” is what a woman says it is.

Until a few months ago, I hadn’t pondered female lust, or consider how it differed from its corporeal cousins, desire and arousal. Go ahead and get your guffaws out of the way. Certainly, someone who makes a life out of exploring intimacy should know the distinctions, you are thinking. I sheepishly admit that until the nuances were brought to my attention, I viewed them interchangeably. Shame on me (in a good shame sort of way).

On the other hand, if you’re intrigued and asking if lust is not sexual desire, then what is it? And which comes first, female arousal or the desire to have sex? And how do these change over the course of a woman’s lifetime? Then you’re in the company of a new breed of sexplorators.

The sexually curious are directing their inquisitive lenses to such things as why women lust in the first place, how our sexuality meanders from “hot” to “not,” and why female genitals tell a different story than self-reported levels of arousal.

University of Texas psychologist Davis Buss compared three groups of women—ages 18 to 26, 26 to 45, and 46 and older—and reported the results earlier this year in the journal, Personality and Individual Difference. He and his colleagues discovered that women in their thirties and forties were significantly more sexual than younger and older females.

This wasn’t surprising news to Susan Crain Bakos, author of several books including The Little Book of the Big Orgasm: More Techniques & Games for Amazing Orgasms Than You Could Possibly Imagine Trying (Quiver, 2010). “[Older women have] more intense sexual fantasies, more orgasms, more sex—and even more one-night stands,” she explains.

Nor is it a great revelation that women have a greater capacity for sexual pleasure or become more sexual once they age past their twenties, prime reproductive years from an evolutionarily point of view. What fascinated us both was the next bit. “Older men presumably continue to lust because they are evolutionarily programmed to keep spreading their genes until they drop dead—but why would women continue to lust, beyond an interest in procreation, beyond minilaproscopy tubal ligation, beyond menopause, outside marriage—and from the male viewpoint perhaps, beyond all reason?”


Why indeed?

Some have suggested that a woman’s hormonal milieu is to blame. In her teens and twenties, a female’s estrogen levels are peaking, coinciding with her most fertile years. It doesn’t really matter if she wants to have sex much, the theory goes, because the odds are generally good that she’ll get pregnant when she does. However, as female hormones decline (starting in the late twenties to early thirties), the proportion of testosterone goes up relatively speaking. A woman responds by becoming hornier, Buss proffers, because she’s unconsciously driven to maximize her chances of getting pregnant while she still can.

All that lust for all things sexual isn’t synonymous with desire, however. This was brought to my attention recently via a scintillating thread on Facebook. Like a verbal voyeur, I read on the sidelines, and learned the following (forgive me, readers, for I have sinned: I cannot repeat those answers verbatim nor can I find them any longer, so I must paraphrase):

Desire refers to our longings, wishes or yearnings for something or someone. Implicit in the context of sex and this discussion is the idea of reciprocity and mutual joy. Lust, on the other hand, is all about ME. It’s uncontrolled, impersonal, and inordinate, a physical need intent on being satisfied without regard for anyone else. The object doesn’t matter, because they are just that … a means to a self-focused end.

This subtle difference points to kinks in the tale of lust and desire. For women—as opposed to men, the prism by which and until recently, most research was viewed through—sexual desire often follows arousal—defined as our body’s physical response to sexual stimuli.

Even if we are technically “turned on,” genitally speaking, it doesn’t mean we are actually in the mood for sex, subjectively speaking.

The relationship between these two: subjective and genital arousal, is strongly predictable in men. Increased blood flow to the genitals leads to an erection, arousal and desire for sex. What’s more, guys are more candid about being turned on.

University of Toronto sexologist Meredith Chivers analyzed over 100 studies measuring arousal, and found a significant correlation between what a man’s penis was saying, and what he’d self-reported. This was true for straight and gay men.

Not so with women of any persuasion. Their loins could be screaming, I’m Ready For Sex! but that’s not what they admitted. Were they lying? Don’t be so quick to jump to that conclusion.

It turns out that for women, being in the mood to make love is as much a matter of the mind, and effected by many things like the state of our relationships, the length of our to-do list, and how we feel about ourselves, physically and sexually speaking (a truncated list, to be sure). This is one reason why some believe that the female equivalent to Viagra™ isn’t going to have much success for the majority of women with diminished female sexual arousal. All the flushing in the world won’t guarantee that a gal’s mind and genitalia are harmonized.

Saturday, 1 January 2011

The Hidden Massacre of Fallujah

This is the terrible testimony given by Jeff Englehart, veteran of the war in Iraq. "I have seen women and children burnt bodies - the former U.S. soldier added - phosphorus explodes and it creates a cloud. Whoever is within 150 mt is dead." Some witnesses have seen a rainfall of burning substances of different colors that were burning people when hit and even those who were not hit had problems breathing", told us Mohamad Tareq al-Deraji, director of the center for human rights studies in Fallujah.